graphic source |
nih | The gap between Whites and Blacks in
levels of violence has animated a prolonged and controversial debate in
public health and the social sciences. Our study reveals that over 60%
of this gap is explained by immigration status, marriage, length of
residence, verbal/reading ability, impulsivity, and neighborhood
context. If we focus on odds ratios rather than raw coefficients, 70% of
the gap is explained. Of all factors, neighborhood context was the most
important source of the gap reduction and constitutional differences
the least important.
We acknowledge the harsh and often
justified criticism that tests of intelligence have endured, but we
would emphasize 2 facts from our findings. First, measured
verbal/reading ability, along with impulsivity/hyperactivity, predicted
violence, in keeping with a long line of prior research.25–28 Second, however, neither factor accounted for much in the way of racial or ethnic disparities
in violence. Whatever the ultimate validity of the constitutional
difference argument, the main conclusion is that its efficacy as an
explainer of race and violence is weak.
Our findings
are consistent with the hypothesis that Blacks are segregated by
neighborhood and thus differentially exposed to key risk and protective
factors, an essential ingredient to understanding the Black–White
disparity in violence.17
The race-related neighborhood features predicting violence are
percentage professional/managerial workers, moral/legal cynicism, and
the concentration of immigration. We found no systematic evidence that
neighborhood- or individual-level predictors of violence interacted with
race/ ethnicity. The relationships we observed thus appeared to be
generally robust across racial/ ethnic groups. We also found no
significant racial or ethnic disparities in trajectories of change in
violence.
Similar to the arguments made by William Julius Wilson in The Truly Disadvantaged,20
these results imply that generic interventions to improve neighborhood
conditions may reduce the racial gap in violence. Policies such as
housing vouchers to aid the poor in securing residence in middle-class
neighborhoods43
may achieve the most effective results in bringing down the
long-standing racial disparities in violence. Policies to increase home
ownership and hence stability of residence may also reduce disparities
(see model 3, Table 2 ).
Family
social conditions matter as well. Our data show that parents being
married, but not family configuration per se, is a salient factor
predicting both the lower probability of violence and a significant
reduction in the Black–White gap in violence. The tendency in past
debates on Black families has been either to pathologize female-headed
households as a singular risk factor or to emphasize the presence of
extended kin as a protective factor. Yet neither factor predicts
violence in our data. Rather, being reared in married-parent households
is the distinguishing factor for children, supporting recent work on the
social influence of marriage44,45 and calls for renewed attention to the labor-market contexts that support stable marriages among the poor.46
Although
the original gap in violence between Whites and Latinos was smaller
than that between Whites and Blacks, our analysis nonetheless explained
the entire gap in violence between Whites and Latino ethnic groups. The
lower rate of violence among Mexican Americans compared with Whites was
explained by a combination of married parents, living in a neighborhood
with a high concentration of immigrants, and individual immigrant
status. The contextual effect of concentrated immigration was robust,
holding up even after a host of factors, including the immigrant status
of the person, were taken into account.
The limitations
of our study raise issues for future research. Perhaps most important
is the need to replicate the results in cities other than Chicago. The
mechanisms explaining the apparent benefits to those living in areas of
concentrated immigration need to be further addressed, and we look to
future research to examine Black–White differences in rates of violence
that remain unexplained. As with any nonexperimental research, it is
also possible we left out key risk factors correlated with race or
ethnicity. Still, to overturn our results any such factors would have to
be correlated with neighborhood characteristics and uncorrelated with
the dozen-plus individual and family background measures, an unlikely
scenario. Even controlling for the criminality of parents did not
diminish the effects of neighborhood characteristics. Finally, it is
possible that family characteristics associated with violence, such as
marital status, were themselves affected by neighborhood residence. If
so, our analysis would mostly likely have underestimated the association
between neighborhood conditions and violence.
0 comments:
Post a Comment